Recovery from acute hypoxia: A systematic review of cognitive and physiological responses during the ‘hypoxia hangover’ (2024)

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Recovery from acute hypoxia: A systematic review of cognitive and physiological responses during the ‘hypoxia hangover’ (1)

Link to Publisher's site

PLoS One. 2023; 18(8): e0289716.

Published online 2023 Aug 16. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0289716

PMCID: PMC10431643

PMID: 37585402

David M. Shaw, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing,Recovery from acute hypoxia: A systematic review of cognitive and physiological responses during the ‘hypoxia hangover’ (2)1,* Peter M. Bloomfield, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing,2 Anthony Benfell, Data curation, Writing – review & editing,3 Isadore Hughes, Data curation, Writing – review & editing,3 and Nicholas Gant, Writing – review & editing2

Vadim Ten, Editor

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement

Abstract

Recovery of cognitive and physiological responses following a hypoxic exposure may not be considered in various operational and research settings. Understanding recovery profiles and influential factors can guide post-hypoxia restrictions to reduce the risk of further cognitive and physiological deterioration, and the potential for incidents and accidents. We systematically evaluated the available evidence on recovery of cognitive and basic physiological responses following an acute hypoxic exposure to improve understanding of the performance and safety implications, and to inform post-hypoxia restrictions. This systematic review summarises 30 studies that document the recovery of either a cognitive or physiological index from an acute hypoxic exposure. Titles and abstracts from PubMed (MEDLINE) and Scopus were searched from inception to July 2022, of which 22 full text articles were considered eligible. An additional 8 articles from other sources were identified and also considered eligible. The overall quality of evidence was moderate (average Rosendal score, 58%) and there was a large range of hypoxic exposures. Heart rate, peripheral blood haemoglobin-oxygen saturation and heart rate variability typically normalised within seconds-to-minutes following return to normoxia or hyperoxia. Whereas, cognitive performance, blood pressure, cerebral tissue oxygenation, ventilation and electroencephalogram indices could persist for minutes-to-hours following a hypoxic exposure, and one study suggested regional cerebral tissue oxygenation requires up to 24 hours to recover. Full recovery of most cognitive and physiological indices, however, appear much sooner and typically within ~2–4 hours. Based on these findings, there is evidence to support a ‘hypoxia hangover’ and a need to implement restrictions following acute hypoxic exposures. The severity and duration of these restrictions is unclear but should consider the population, subsequent requirement for safety-critical tasks and hypoxic exposure.

Introduction

Hypoxia is a state of insufficient oxygen which can compromise normal physiological and cognitive functions, and manifests when breathing air with a lower partial pressure of oxygen (PO2) compared to sea-level (i.e. <159 mmHg) [1, 2]. The initial compensatory responses to the resulting hypoxaemia (i.e. low arterial partial pressure of oxygen) include cardiopulmonary, respiratory and metabolic, which aim to maintain oxygen supply to vital tissues, but tissues eventually desaturate when PO2 is sufficiently low. The brain’s high rates of oxidative metabolism (20–25% resting metabolic rate) make it vulnerable to oxygen depletion [3] and energetically-demanding cognitive functions are easily impaired during hypoxic exposures [1, 4]. Temporal recovery from hypoxia is assumed to be rapid since peripheral blood haemoglobin reoxygenates within seconds-to-minutes; however, cognitive and physiological perturbations can persist beyond the recovery of blood and tissue oxygenation [5]; a state that has been colloquially termed the ‘hypoxia hangover’ [6].

The recovery profiles of cognitive and physiological responses from an acute hypoxic exposure have performance and safety implications for various operational and research populations. For example, military aviators undertake hypoxia recognition training at least once every five years and are prohibited from flying duties for the following 12–24 hours. Recent studies by the Naval Medical Research Unit (Dayton, Ohio, USA), however, have demonstrated cognitive and physiological indices fully recover almost immediately [7] or within 2–4 hours [5] following a hypoxic exposure, which suggests the grounding period for military aviators could be reduced. If post-hypoxia restrictions are implemented, the influence of the hypoxic dose (barometric pressure, fraction of inspired oxygen [FIO2] and duration of exposure), recovery procedures (e.g. normoxic or hyperoxic breathing) and safety-critical nature of subsequent tasks should also be considered. There is a need to evaluate if post-hypoxia restrictions are required for different populations and to establish clear evidence-based recommendations that inform operational, training and research scenarios.

Therefore, we conducted a systematic review examining the recovery profiles of cognitive and physiological responses following an acute hypoxic exposure in healthy individuals. The aim was to determine whether post-hypoxia restrictions should be implemented, evaluate if the hypoxic dose or recovery procedures influence cognitive and basic physiological indices, and identify gaps in knowledge for future investigations. Outcomes will be crucial for populations that experience hypoxia during training or operational duty (e.g. military aviators), and to manage participants of research studies evaluating the effects of hypoxic interventions.

Methodology

We followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) 2020 guidelines [8]. This review was not preregistered as it stemmed from a project to inform recommendations following normobaric and hypobaric hypoxia recognition training of military aviators within the Royal New Zealand Air Force.

Search

We searched titles and abstracts from PubMed (MEDLINE) and Scopus from inception to July 2022 for potential research studies using the search terms and Boolean operators: (hypoxia or hypoxic) AND (hangover OR recovery) AND (cognition OR cognitive OR "cognitive performance" OR “flight performance” OR mood OR sleepiness OR symptoms OR effort OR physiology OR physiological OR oxygenation OR "oxygen saturation" OR "heart rate" OR "heart rate variability" OR “blood pressure” OR ventilation OR “respiratory rate” OR respiration OR “blood gases”) NOT (animal OR murine OR rodent OR mice OR porcine OR dog OR fish OR cell). Furthermore, additional articles known to the first author (DMS) that were not identified in the literature search were added and titles (only) of the following were screened by DMS: 1) reference lists of eligible articles; 2) forward citation tracking using Google Scholar of eligible articles; and 3) searching key journals and Google Scholar using a combination of the terms: hypoxia, hypoxic, recovery and hangover. All potential articles identified through other sources were screened by DMS immediately following retrieval. Full details of the screening process are displayed in Fig 1.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

We included studies fulfilling the following criteria: 1) between- or within-subject experimental trials; 2) human participants of any age with no known medical conditions; 3) an objective measure of cognition or physiology during recovery from an acute hypoxic exposure; and 4) peer reviewed full text original research studies published in English. Acute hypoxia was defined as <300 min of breathing hypoxic air that lowered peripheral blood haemoglobin-oxygen saturation (SpO2) to <90% (i.e. normobaric or hypobaric hypoxia) and recovery was defined as the period of breathing air that increased and normalised peripheral blood SpO2 to >95% following an acute hypoxic exposure. We excluded studies if: 1) the control or baseline group (or condition) were not administered air able to maintain SpO2 >95%; 2) other interventions known to effect cognition were included in conjunction with oxygen manipulation, except for carbon dioxide; 3) single or accumulated repeated hypoxia exposures were administered beyond 300 min; 4) hypoxia was repeatedly interrupted by normoxic breathing; and 5) cognitive and/or physiological data were not adequately reported. As we were specifically interested in physiological responses that can be readily measured in military aviation training and operational settings, we restricted our physiological indices of interest to ventilatory, cardiovascular, regional cerebral tissue haemoglobin-oxygen saturation (rSO2), SpO2, and cardiac autonomic responses. Measures of less interest and low feasibility in the training and operational setting (e.g. intra-ocular pressure, limb blood flow, and vascular resistance) were omitted. Review articles, unpublished abstracts, theses, and dissertations were also excluded.

Screening

Literature search results were entered into Mendeley, which automatically removed duplicates, then exported to Rayyan (i.e. online systematic review software). Two authors (DMS and AB) independently screened titles and abstracts for suitability. The full text for studies of interest were retrieved and independently evaluated for suitability by the same two authors. Disagreements between authors’ decisions were resolved via discussion and consensus.

Risk of bias/quality assessment

Two authors (DMS and PMB) independently performed the assessment of risk of bias in the included studies using the Rosendal Scale [9]. This scale combines the PEDro scale [10], Jadad scoring system [11] and Delphi list [12]. This Rosendal scale was selected as the PEDro scale, Jadad scoring system and Delphi list have been extensively evaluated and validated. A Rosendal score of 60% is considered as excellent methodological quality [9]. Two checklist items were removed, including to whether the researchers were blinded as it was deemed inappropriate (i.e. due to safety reasons) and reporting of methods used to report adverse effects as the responses to acute hypoxia are inherently deemed adverse. An additional checklist item assessing whether treatment order was counterbalanced was included. No studies were excluded based on quality assessment results. Disagreements were resolved by discussion.

Results

Overview of included studies and study quality

The literature search initially identified 2698 studies for screening. The full text of 32 studies were assessed and 22 were considered eligible. An additional 8 studies from other sources were identified and considered eligible, giving a total of 30 full text articles included in this review. Table 1 provides an overview of hypoxia and recovery interventions for the included studies. 20 studies utilised poikilocapnic hypoxia [5, 6, 1330], 9 studies utilised isocapnic hypoxia [3139] and 1 study utilised hypercapnic hypoxia [40]. The duration of hypoxia was typically <30 min and depended on the severity of hypoxic breathing; for instance, studies assessing ~25,000 ft (7,600 m) equivalent (i.e. ~7–8% oxygen) were ~5 min or less, whereas, studies assessing ~18,000 ft (5,486 m) equivalent (i.e. ~10–11% oxygen) were ~25–30 min. The severity of hypoxic breathing was generally >18,000 ft equivalent or <10–11% oxygen, and some studies titrated the FIO2 to maintain a specific end-tidal partial pressure of oxygen (PETO2) or SpO2. Some studies assessed different hypoxic durations and severities. For recovery, 19 studies only administered normoxia (i.e. 21% oxygen) [14, 15, 2027, 30, 3234, 3640], 2 studies only administered hyperoxia [18, 29], 5 studies administered both normoxia and hyperoxia [6, 13, 17, 28, 35], and 4 studies compared differences between normoxic and hyperoxic interventions [5, 16, 19, 31]. We included measures of cognitive performance, SpO2, heart rate (HR), heart rate variability (HRV; specifically, root mean square of successive differences between normal heartbeats [rMSSD], standard deviation of NN intervals [SDNN] and low-frequency [LF]/high-frequency [HF] ratio of spectral domains), blood pressure (BP; specifically mean arterial pressure [MAP], diastolic blood pressure [DBP] and systolic blood pressure [SBP]), rSO2, ventilation and electroencephalogram (EEG) measurements. Methodological quality assessment yielded an average Rosendal score of 58 ± 15% (range, 30–87%). Full results of the quality assessment can be found in S1 Table.

Table 1

Characteristics of included studies reporting recovery of cognitive and/or physiological responses following a hypoxic exposure.

ReferencePopulationHypoxic and Recovery Interventions
Bascom et al. (1992)n = 6 (4 men, 2 women), mean age = 24.5, healthy adultsMethod: Mouth-piece
Baseline: 10 min of normobaric O2 to PETO2 of 100 mmHg, with PETCO2 held at 1–2 mmHg above resting
Hypoxia: 20 min of reduced normobaric O2 to PETO2 of 45, 50, 55, 65 or 75 mmHg, with PETCO2 held at 1–2 mmHg above resting
Recovery: 5 min of normobaric O2 to PETO2 of 100 mmHg, with PETCO2 held at 1–2 mmHg above resting
Note: The second hypoxia period was omitted.
Beer et al. (2017)n = 11 (9 men, 3 women; n = 1 dropout but sex not specified), healthy military personnelMethod: Hypobaric chamber and mask
Baseline: 15 min of normobaric 21% O2, then ascent to 10,000 ft at 5,000 ft/min, 9 min of 10,000 ft, then
30 min of 100% O2 at 10,000 ft
Hypoxia: Ascent to 18,000 ft or 25,000 ft at 5000 ft/min, then maintained for 20 min or (typically) <5 min, respectively
Recovery: 100% O2 during descent to ground level at 5000 ft/min for SpO2 >90%, then 15 min of normobaric 21% O2
Note: Mask-on only during 100% O2
Blacker et al. (2021)n = 26 (14 men, 12 women; n = 3 excluded due to noisy EEG data but sex not specified), mean age = 30.5, healthy Air Force personnelMethod: Reduced oxygen breathing environment and mask
Baseline: 30 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 10 min normobaric 9.7% O2 (~20,000 ft equivalent)
Recovery: 30 min normobaric 21% or 100% oxygen, then 210 min (mask-off) normobaric 21% O2
Botek et al. (2015)n = 29, men, mean age = 26, healthy sports science studentsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 7 min (mask off) normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 10 min normobaric 9.6% O2 (~20,341 ft equivalent)
Recovery: 7 min (mask-off) normobaric 21% O2
Botek et al. (2018)n = 58 (28 men, 30 women), mean age = 23.8, healthy studentsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 6 min (mask off) normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 10 min normobaric 9.6% (~20,341 ft equivalent)
Recovery: 7 min normobaric 21% O2
Dahan et al. (1995)n = 10 (6 men, 4 women), age range = 23 to 32, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 15–20 min normobaric O2 to PETO2 109 mmHg (i.e. normoxic) or normobaric O2 to PETO2 525 mmHg (i.e. hyperoxic)
Hypoxia: 30 sec or 1, 3, and 5 min reduced normobaric O2 to PETO2 49 mmHg
Recovery: 3–5 min normobaric 21% O2 (3 and 5 min hypoxic exposures were administered an additional 10 min normobaric 70% O2) or 3–5 min hyperoxia (PETO2 525 mmHg)
Dart et al. (2017)n = 10, men, mean age = 31.4, healthy military personnelMethod: Hypobaric chamber and mask
Baseline: 15 min normobaric 100% O2 prior to the 10,000 ft and 15,000 ft exposures and 30 min normobaric 100% O2 prior to the 20,000 ft exposure
Hypoxia: ~60 min, ~45 min, and ~20 min at 10,000 ft, 15,000 ft, and 20,000 ft, respectively
Recovery: 100% O2 during descent to ground level, then 1 hour (mask-off) 21% normobaric O2
Note: Mask-on only during 100% O2
Easton et al. (1988)n = 11 (5 men, 6 women), mean age = 25, healthy adultsMethod: Mouth-piece
Baseline: 6–8 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 25 min normobaric 8–10% O2 to SpO2 of ~80% with PETCO2 held constant to baseline
Recovery: Either; 1) 7 min normobaric 21% O2; 2) 15 min normobaric 21% O2; 3) 60 min normobaric 21% O2; 4) 7 min normobaric 100% O2; or 5) 15 min normobaric 30% O2,
Note: The second hypoxic period was omitted
Georgopoulos et al. (1990)n = 8 (5 men, 3 women), mean age = 28, healthy adultsMethod: Mouth-piece
Baseline: 8–10 min normobaric 21% O2, then 5 min 21% O2 with added CO2 (i.e. FICO2 of 4.5–5%; PETCO2 5–14 mmHg), then 5 min 21% O2
Hypoxia: 25 min normobaric 8–9% O2 to SpO2 of ~80% (or 21% O2) with PETCO2 held constant to baseline
Recovery: 5 min normobaric 21% O2, then 5 min 21% O2 with added CO2 (i.e. CO2 titrated to match PETCO2 of initial hypercapnic exposure), then 5 min 21% O2
Harshman et al. (2015)n = 8, men, mean age = 29.9, healthy military personnelMethod: Mask
Baseline: 5 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 5 min normobaric 8% O2
Recovery: 5 min normobaric 100% O2
Janaky et al. (2007)n = 14, men, mean age = 39.7, health adults, either candidates for pilot school or lab personnelMethod: Hypobaric chamber
Baseline: 20 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 15 min at 5500 m (18,044 ft)
Recovery: 20 min normobaric 21% O2
Malle et al. (2016)n = 42, men, mean age = 29.4, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 30 sec normobaric 21% O2 or 100% O2 (normoxia included 10 min prior 21% O2 and hyperoxia included 3 min prior 100% O2)
Hypoxia: ~3 min normobaric 6% O2 (or 21% O2)
Recovery: 10 min normobaric 21% O2 or 3 min 100% O2 then 21% O2
Note: n = 22 in Hypoxia-Air group and n = 20 in Hypoxia-O2 group; comparisons between group and to baseline.
Morgan et al. (1995)n = 10, men, mean age = ~37, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: Normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 20 min reduced normobaric O2 to SpO2 80% and increased CO2 to PETCO2 to +5 mmHg of baseline (control group of n = 5 breathed 21% O2 for 40 min)
Recovery: 20 min normobaric 21% O2
Najmanová et al. (2019)n = 38 (15 men, 23 women), mean age = ~25, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 7 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 10 min normobaric 9.6% O2 (~20,341 ft equivalent)
Recovery: 7 min normobaric 21% O2
Phillips et al. (2009)n = 36, healthy military aviation personnelMethod: Mask
Baseline: 10 min 21% O2
Hypoxia (profile 1, n = 9): 3 rapid (2–3 s) transitions to normobaric 9.1, 11.4 and 14.1% O2 with rapid return to 21% O2 between each (i.e. 5 min at altitude and 5 min at 21% O2), then gradual transition (4 min) to normobaric 9.1% O2 and return to 21% O2
Hypoxia (profile 2, n = 7): Gradual transition (4 min) to normobaric 9.1% O2 and return to 21% O2, then 3 rapid (2–3 s) transitions to normobaric 9.1, 14.1, and 11.4% O2 with return to 21% O2 between each (i.e. 5 min at altitude and 5 min at 21% O2)
Note: n = 20 were in the control (normoxic) group.
Recovery: Gradual (4 min) transition to normobaric 21% O2, then maintained for 10 min
Phillips et al. (2015)n = 19, healthy active duty military personnelMethod: Mask
Baseline: 21% normobaric O2 on prior visit
Hypoxia: 30 min (or until SpO2 dropped below 50%) normobaric 9.96% O2 (~18,000 ft equivalent)
Recovery: 24 hr normobaric 21% O2
Querido et al. (2010)n = 7 (5 men, 2 women), mean age = 30, healthyMethod: Mask
Baseline: ≥15 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 20 min reduced O2 to SpO2 80% and increased CO2 to PETCO2 to baseline levels
Recovery: ~4.5 min normobaric 21%, then 1 min normobaric 100% O2 (termed “intervention” and repeated 3 times)
Querido et al. (2011)n = 14 (11 men, 3 women), mean age = 26, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 15 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 20 min of reduced normobaric O2 to SpO2 of 80% with PETCO2 maintained at baseline levels
Recovery: ~5 min normobaric 21% O2 (n = 15) and 20 min normobaric 21% O2 (n = 5)
Robinson et al. (2018)n = 21 (20 men, 1 women), age range = 22–37, healthy Air Force personnel (non-pilots)Method: Mask
Baseline: 5 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 5 min normobaric 6.5% O2 (~25,000 ft equivalent)
Recovery: 5 min normobaric 21% O2 followed by 30 min normobaric 21% or 13.1% O2 (~10,000 ft equivalent)
Note: Comparisons with baseline were not possible due to methodological issues
Roche et al. (2002)n = 11 (6 men, 5 women), mean age = 29, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 20 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 15 min normobaric 11% O2 (~15,748 ft equivalent)
Recovery: 20 min normobaric 21% O2
Sausen et al. (2003)n = 4 (3 men, 1 women), mean age = 24.7, healthy US student Naval flight surgeonsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 15 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 20 min transition to normobaric 10% O2 (~18,000 ft equivalent), then 30 min 10% O2, then 5 min transition to normobaric 21% O2
Recovery: 15 min normobaric 21% O2
Steinback et al. (2012)n = 7 (3 men, 4 women), mean age = 27, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 10 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: Step decrease in normobaric O2 to PETO2 of 45 mmHg (~80% SpO2) and maintenance of baseline PETCO2, then maintained for 5 min
Recovery: 10 min normobaric 21% O2
Stepanek et al. (2013)n = 25 (14 men, 11 women), mean age = 32.4, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: Normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: ~4–5 min normobaric 8% O2 (~23,300 ft equivalent)
Recovery: ~4–5 min normobaric 21% O2
Stepanek et al. (2014)n = 25 (14 men, 11 women), mean age = 32.4, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
2 profiles separated by a 6 min washout period
Baseline: Normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 3 min of either normobaric 8% O2 or normobaric 7% O2 + 5% CO2
Recovery: 3 min normobaric 21% O2
Tamisier et al. (2005)n = 10 (6 men, 4 women), mean age = 29.8, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: Normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 2 hr normobaric ~9% O2 to ~85% SpO2
Recovery: Normobaric 21% O2
Uchida et al. (2020)n = 9 (2 men, 7 women), mean age = 28, healthy adultsMethod: Mask
Baseline: 7 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 7–10 min normobaric 11.8% O2 (~15,00 ft equivalent)
Recovery: 13–16 min normobaric 21% O2
Varis et al. (2019)n = 16, men, healthy qualified Hawk pilotsMethod: Mask
3 profiles separated by 10 min washout periods within ~40 min
Baseline: N/A
Hypoxia: Pressurised air, then ~123 sec, ~93 sec, and ~115 sec of normobaric 8% (~20,341 ft equivalent), 7% (~22,966 ft equivalent) or 6% O2 (~25,919 ft equivalent), respectively
Recovery: 100% normobaric O2 until emergency procedure completion; return to base flight 10 min after emergency procedures completion
Note: Performance was compared to control flight with normobaric 21% O2 (not hypoxia) following the 6% O2 condition only; 60 minutes rest between trials
Varis et al. (2022)n = 15, men, mean age = 24.6, healthy Hawk fighter pilotsMethod: Mask
Baseline: N/A
Hypoxia: ~75 sec normobaric 6% O2 or ~103 sec normobaric 8% O2
Recovery: ~58 sec normobaric 100% O2 following 6% O2 and ~42 sec normobaric 100% O2 following 8% O2 (to complete emergency procedures); return to base flight 10 min after emergency procedures completion
Note: Performance was compared to control flight with normobaric 21% O2 (not hypoxia)
Vigo et al. (2010)n = 12, men, mean age = 28, healthy military pilotsMethod: Hypobaric chamber and mask
Baseline: ~89 sec 100% O2 during ascent to 27,000 ft
Hypoxia: ~113 sec at 27,000 ft
Recovery: ~137 sec 100% O2 at 27,000 ft
Xie et al. (2001)SpO2, HR, BP and HRV: n = 11 (7 men, 4 women), mean age = 26, healthy adults
Ventilation: n = 9 (6 men, 3 women), mean age = 29, healthy adults
Method: Mask
Baseline: 10 min normobaric 21% O2
Hypoxia: 20 min normobaric 10–12% O2 to lower SpO2 to ~80% and maintain baseline PETCO2
Recovery: 20 min normobaric 21% O2

The estimated altitude equivalent is reported for studies using normobaric hypoxia that reported targeting a specific altitude. Altitude equivalents are reported in ft in line with aviation standards; for reference: 5,000 ft = 1,524 m, 10,000 ft = 3,048 m, 18,000 ft = 5,486 m, and 25,000 ft = 7,620 m. Abbreviations: O2 = oxygen; SpO2 = peripheral blood haemoglobin-oxygen saturation; PETO2 = end-tidal partial pressure of oxygen; CO2 = carbon dioxide; FICO2 = fraction of inspired carbon dioxide; PETCO2 = end-tidal partial pressure of carbon dioxide.

Cognition

Eleven studies measured cognition during recovery from hypoxia, including measures from standardised tests and simulated flight performance (Table 2). One study measured composite scores of different cognitive domains within the synthetic work environment (SYNWIN) test battery and reported normalisation at ~20 min [13]. Three studies measured simple and choice reaction time; 1 study reported normalisation at 60 min [17] and 2 studies reported continued impairments, 1 at 10 min [21] and 1 at 24 hours [20]. Two studies measured attention; 1 study reported normalisation of reaction time and lapses (using the psychom*otor vigilance task [PVT]) at 60 min [5] and 1 study reported normalisation of commission errors (using the Conners’ continuous performance test) at 13–16 min [27]. One study measured sequential number reading and reported normalisation at 5 min [25] and 1 study measured mathematical processing using the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT), a proxy of attention and working memory, and reported normalisation at 90 sec [19]. Two studies measured simulated flight performance and reported impairments for 10 min [6, 28]. Another study also measured simulated flight performance, in conjunction with a time-estimation task, but due to methodological issues, could not make comparisons to baseline; instead, comparisons between 21% and 13.1% oxygen recovery revealed no difference in flight performance errors, and time-estimation task lapses and variance at 35 min [22]. Not all hypoxic protocols impaired all measures of cognition performance [5, 20, 27] and 21% versus 100% oxygen did not elicit clear or persistent differences in cognitive performance [5, 19].

Table 2

Studies reporting recovery of cognitive responses following a hypoxic exposure.

ReferenceCognitive taskEffect of hypoxiaRecovery from hypoxia
Beer et al. (2017)SYNWIN test battery (short-term memory, mathematical addition, visual monitoring, auditory monitoring)Reduced composite test battery scores (18,000 ft scores <25,000 ft scores)Composite test battery scores normalised at ~20 min
Blacker et al. (2021)10 min psychom*otor vigilance task (median reaction time and minor [500–1000 ms] and major [>1000 ms] lapses)Slower median reaction time and (probably) increased minor lapses but no effect for major lapsesMedian reaction time and minor lapses normalised at 60 min for 21% and 100% O2 (slower at 20 min)
Dart et al. (2017)Simple and choice reaction time tasksSlower total response time for simple and choice reaction time tasks at 15,000 and 20,000 ft but not 10,000 ft; negligible differences in accuracy for simple and choice reaction time tasksTotal response time for simple and choice reaction tasks normalised at 60 min
Malle et al. (2016)PASAT (attentional processing and working memory)Reduced correct responses, increased omissions, increased errors and increased miscalculations*Correct responses, omissions, errors and miscalculations normalised at 80 sec for 21% and 100% O2; however, 100% O2 had initial transient lower correct responses and increased omissions compared to 21% O2
Phillips et al. (2009)Flanker arrow task (two choice reaction time i.e. response conflict alone and stimulus + response conflict) (during the 9.1% O2trial)Slower 2-choice reaction timeSlower 2-choice reaction time at 10 min
Phillips et al. (2015)Cognitive/Perceptual test battery (Number Stroop Task, reaction time, choice reaction time, NASA-TLX)Slower simple and choice reaction time, increased task effort but no effect for Number Stroop taskSimple and choice reaction time normalised at 24 hours (60 and 120 min remained increased) and task effort normalised immediately
Robinson et al. (2018)Simulator flight performance and time estimation taskIncreased flight performance errors, time-estimation task lapses and variance*No difference between 13.1% and 21% O2 for flight performance errors, and time-estimation task lapses and variance at 35 min
Stepanek et al. (2013)King-Devick test (sequential rapid number reading)Slower task completion and increased errorsTask time and errors normalised at 5 min
Uchida et al. (2020)Conners’ continuous performance test–(reaction time and commission and omission errors)Increased commission errors but no effect on reaction time or omission errorsCommission errors normalised at 13–16 min
Varis et al. (2019)Simulator flight performanceUnclear, but likely impaired situational awareness and flight performanceImpaired situational awareness and flight performance at ~10 min; adverse subjective effects reported up to 12 hours after, such as fatigue, tiredness, dizziness and headaches
Varis et al. (2022)Simulator flight performanceUnclear, but likely impaired flight performanceImpaired flight performance following 6% (but not 8%) O2 at ~10 min; adverse subjective effects reported immediately after, such as light headedness, visual impairments and dizziness

Effects are compared to a normoxic baseline (i.e. either immediately before the hypoxic intervention or during a separate trial), unless otherwise indicated.

*Comparisons were between conditions (not to baseline). Altitude equivalents are reported in ft in line with aviation standards; for reference: 5,000 ft = 1,524 m, 10,000 ft = 3,048 m, 18,000 ft = 5,486 m, and 25,000 ft = 7,620 m. Abbreviations: SYNWIN = Synthetic work environment; PASAT = Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task; O2 = oxygen; NASA-TLX = National Aeronautics and Space Administration-Task Load Index.

Cardiovascular responses

Table 3 provides an overview of the recovery of SpO2, HR, HRV and BP during recovery from hypoxia. Eighteen studies measured SpO2; SpO2 normalised within 1–10 minutes in almost all studies [5, 14, 15, 18, 19, 21, 2426, 29, 3437] and three studies reported SpO2 normalised by 13–35 min [23, 27, 30]. One study could only report SpO2 recovery compared to a hypoxic condition [22]. Fifteen studies measured HR; HR recovered in 1–10 min in nine studies [5, 19, 24, 29, 33, 35, 36, 38, 39] and 11–20 min in three studies [26, 27]. HR was lower at 7 min in two studies [14, 15] and one study could only report HR recovery compared to a hypoxic condition [22]. Five studies measured HRV indices; HRV normalised at 3–7 min in three studies [14, 15, 19] and at 20 min in one study [23], whereas one study reported some perturbations of HRV persisted at ~137 sec [29]. Seven studies measured BP indices; BP normalised at 5–10 min in 3 studies (i.e. SBP [33, 38] and MAP and SBP [39]), and SBP and DBP normalised at 20 min in one study [30]. However, SBP remained increased after an unspecified time in one study [35], DBP and SBP remained increased at 5 min in one study [36], and MAP remained increased at 11 min in one study [26]. HR, HRV and BP indices were not altered by all hypoxic interventions, and 100% oxygen breathing during recovery normalised SpO2 [5, 19] and HR [5] only marginally faster (seconds) than 21% oxygen.

Table 3

Studies reporting recovery of cardiovascular responses following a hypoxic exposure.

ReferenceEffect of hypoxiaRecovery from hypoxia
Blacker et al. (2021)Reduced SpO2 and increased HRSpO2 normalised at 2 min with 100% O2 and 5 min with 21% O2; HR normalised at 2 min with 21% and 100% O2
Botek et al. (2015)Reduced SpO2, increased HR and increased (natural logarithm) LF/HF ratioSpO2 and (natural logarithm) LF/HF HRV normalised and HR reduced at 7 min
Botek et al. (2018)Reduced SpO2, increased HR, reduced log rMSSD and SDNN (both sexes), increased LF/HF (males only)SpO2 normalised at 7 min in females, but remained reduced in males; HR reduced at 7 min in both sexes; log rMSSD (both sexes), SDNN (males only) and LF/HF (both sexes) normalised at 7 min
Harshman et al. (2015)Reduced SpO2SpO2 normalised at ~1 min
Janáky et al. (2007)Reduced SpO2, increased SBP and DBP, but no effect for HRSpO2,, SBP and DBP normalised at 20 min
Malle et al. (2016)Reduced SpO2, increased HR and reduced HRV*SpO2, HR and HRV normalised at ~3 min; albeit SpO2 increased more rapidly with 100% O2
Morgan et al. (1995)Increased HR and SBP, but no effect for DBPHR and SBP normalised at 5 min
Najmanová (2019)Reduced SpO2 (greater reduction in women)SpO2 normalised at 7 min
Phillips et al. (2009)Reduced SpO2SpO2 normalised at ~1 min
Querido et al. (2010)Reduced SpO2 and increased HR and SBP, but no effect for DBP and MAPSpO2 and HR normalised at ~5 min (i.e. with 21% O2), but SBP remained increased at ~15 min (i.e. following 3rd intervention period) (no effect of hyperoxia vs normoxia)
Querido et al. (2011)Reduced SpO2 and increased HR, MAP, SBP and DBPSpO2 and HR normalised at 5 min, and MAP, SBP and DBP remained increased at 5 min
Roche et al. (2002)Increased HR and LF/HF ratio, and reduced SpO2, but no effect for SBP, DBP and mean BPHR, LF/HF ratio and SpO2 normalised at 20 min
Robinson et al. (2018)Reduced SpO2 and increased HR*SpO2 lower and HR higher in 13.1% compared with 21% following hypoxia at 35 min; no difference in SpO2 or HR during 13.1% O2 when preceded by hypoxia or normoxia
Sausen et al. (2003)Reduced SpO2 and increased HR, but no effect for MAP, SBP and DBPSpO2 normalised at 2 min and HR normalised at 6 min
Steinback et al. (2012)Increased HR, MAP and SBP, but no effect for DBPHR, MAP and SBP normalised at 10 min
Stepanek et al. (2013)Reduced SpO2SpO2 normalised at 2 min
Stepanek et al. (2014)Reduced SpO2SpO2 normalised at 3 min following both exposures
Tamisier et al. (2005)Reduced SpO2 (BP and HR not measured)SpO2 normalised at 1 min; BP increased at 11 min; HR normalised at 11 min
Uchida et al. (2020)Reduced SpO2 and increased HR, but no effect for MAPSpO2 and HR normalised at 13–16 min
Vigo et al. (2010)Reduced SpO2, increased HR, reduced SDNN, but no effect for rMSSD and LF/HFSpO2 and HR normalised at ~137 sec, but SDNN remained reduced
Xie et al. (2001)Increased HR and SBP, but no effect for DBPHR normalised at 5 min and SBP normalised at 10 min

Effects are compared to a normoxic baseline (i.e. either immediately before the hypoxic intervention or during a separate trial), unless otherwise indicated.

*Comparisons were between conditions (not to baseline). Abbreviations: SpO2 = peripheral blood haemoglobin oxygen saturation; HR = heart rate; HRV = heart rate variability; SDNN = standard deviation of RR intervals; rMSSD = root mean square of successive differences between normal heartbeats; LF = low-frequency power; HF = high-frequency power; MAP = mean arterial pressure; SBP = systolic blood pressure; DBP = diastolic blood pressure.

Ventilatory responses

Ten studies measured ventilatory indices during recovery from hypoxia (Table 4). Minute ventilation (VE) normalised within seconds to 15 min in almost all studies [16, 27, 3133, 35, 36, 38, 39]; however, one study reported VE was reduced after 5 min [40] and one study reported VE during hyperoxic recovery transiently undershot hyperoxic baseline following 3 and 5 min of hypoxia (but not shorter hypoxia exposures) [16].

Table 4

Studies reporting recovery of ventilatory responses following a hypoxic exposure.

ReferenceEffect of hypoxiaRecovery from hypoxia
Bascom et al. (1992)Increased VESlightly reduced VE at 5 min for all hypoxia levels
Dahan et al. (1995)Increased VEVE normalised at 69, 54, 12 and 12 sec when 30 sec and 1, 3 and 5 min of hypoxia, respectively, were followed by 21% O2; hyperoxic VE normalised (relative to hyperoxic baseline) at 9, 15, 12 and 9 sec following 30 sec and 1, 3 and 5 min of hypoxia, respectively; hyperoxic VE transiently undershot hyperoxic baseline following 3 and 5 min of hypoxia
Easton et al. (1988)Increased VEVE normalised at 60 min (almost normalised at 15 min) for 21% O2, 15 min for 30% O2 and 7 min for 100% O2
Georgopoulos et al. (1990)Increased VE and tidal volume, but no effect for breathing frequencyVE and tidal volume normalised at 5 min (hypoxia did not affect the ventilatory response to hypercapnia)
Morgan et al. (1995)Increased VEVE normalised at 5 min
Querido et al. (2010)Increased VE and tidal volume, but no effect for breathing frequencyVE and tidal volume normalised at ~5 min (i.e. with 21% O2)
Querido et al. (2011)Increased VE and breathing frequency, but no effect for tidal volumeVE and breathing frequency normalised at 5 min
Steinback et al. (2012)Increased VE and tidal volume, but no effect for breathing frequencyVE and tidal volume normalised at 10 min
Uchida et al. (2020)Increased VEVE normalised at 13–16 min
Xie et al. (2001)Increased VEVE normalised at 5 min

Effects are compared to a normoxic baseline (i.e. either immediately before the hypoxic intervention or during a separate trial), unless otherwise indicated. Abbreviations: VE = minute ventilation; O2 = oxygen.

Neurophysiological responses

Cerebral tissue oxygenation

Four studies measured rSO2 during recovery from hypoxia (Table 5). rSO2 normalised within 10–13 min in two studies [27, 39], whereas one study reported rSO2 did not normalise after 10 min [20] and one study reported rSO2 normalised at 24 hours, but not at 2 hours [20].

Table 5

Studies reporting recovery of regional cerebral tissue oxygenation responses following a hypoxic exposure.

ReferenceEffect of hypoxiaRecovery from hypoxia
Phillips et al. (2009)Reduced rSO2Reduced rSO2 at 10 min
Phillips et al. (2015)Reduced rSO2rSO2 normalised at 24 hours (60 and 120 min remained reduced)
Steinback et al. (2012)Reduced rSO2rSO2 normalised at 10 min
Uchida et al. (2020)Reduced rSO2rSO2 normalised at 13–16 min

Effects of hypoxia on regional cerebral tissue haemoglobin oxygen saturation (rSO2) are compared to a normoxic baseline (i.e. either immediately before the hypoxic intervention or during a separate trial), unless otherwise indicated.

EEG

Two studies measured EEG indices during recovery from hypoxia (Table 6). One study reported differences in EEG indices persisted for up to 4 hours with no difference between 21% and 100% oxygen recovery [5], whereas another study reported rapid normalisation (minutes) of EEG indices albeit a transient (seconds) worsening with 100% oxygen breathing during recovery compared with 21% oxygen [19].

Table 6

Studies reporting recovery of EEG responses following a hypoxic exposure.

ReferenceEffect of hypoxiaRecovery from hypoxia
Blacker et al. (2021)Reduced MMN mean amplitude; no effect for MMN peak latency; increased P3a mean amplitude; no effect for P3a peak latencyMMN mean amplitude normalised at 120 min (reduced at 0, 20 and 60 min); shorter MMN peak latencies normalised at 240 min (reduced at 20, 60, 120 and 180 min); P3a mean amplitude normalised immediately for 21% and 100% O2 with no differences between 21% and 100% O2
Malle et al. (2016)Overall, no effect for EEG spectral power for all groups; however, SEF95 increased*Transient increase in slow-wave activity for both groups, including increased delta wave activity (first 16 sec, both groups) and theta wave activity (first 32 sec in 100% O2); SEF95 transiently increased in 21% O2 and decreased in 100% O2, with SEF95 lower in 100% compared with 21% O2 during first 32 sec and from 64–80

Effects are compared to a normoxic baseline (i.e. either immediately before the hypoxic intervention or during a separate trial), unless otherwise indicated.

*Comparisons were between conditions (not to baseline). Abbreviations: MMN = mismatch negativity component; SEF95 = the 95th percentile spectral edge frequency (i.e. the frequency below which 95% of total electroencephalogram power was contained); EEG = electroencephalogram; O2 = oxygen.

Discussion

State and limitations of evidence

The purpose of this systematic review was to consolidate the available evidence on the recovery of cognitive and basic physiological responses following an acute hypoxic exposure. This was to improve our understanding of how hypoxia could impair performance and compromise safety despite returning to normoxic (or hyperoxic) air breathing. Currently, there are insufficient published articles to accurately quantify post-hypoxia recovery profiles of cognitive and physiological indices, and their influential factors, which prevented meta-analysing the data and restricted this article to a systematic review. Some studies also employed methodologies that made it difficult to extract true recovery durations and, therefore, the time point for full recovery could not be determined. This was often due to articles not reporting time course profiles (i.e. repeated measures) and only reporting a single value that represented a range of time. The majority of research (19 studies) included men and women to allow findings to be applicable to both sexes; however, there was still a tendency to favour males. Study quality was moderate (average Rosendal score of 58 ± 15) and methodological differences made it difficult to compare between studies. Nevertheless, there is sufficient evidence supporting the presence of a ‘hypoxia hangover’ and to help to inform post-hypoxia restrictions.

Recovery of cognitive functions

Hypoxia exponentially degrades cognitive functions with increasing hypoxaemia until loss-of-consciousness [4]. During hypoxia, there is preferential blood flow to posterior regions of the brain that are essential for regulating vital functions (e.g. breathing) [41] but are not highly involved in complex cognitive functions. Therefore, the anterior regions are more at risk of reduced oxygen delivery and, since they are involved in higher order and more complex cognitive processes, are easily degraded by hypoxia. This may be exacerbated by increased oxygen requirement of active brain regions and higher neuronal sensitivity to oxygen deprivation. In the present review, studies included a range of simple and complex cognitive tasks, and since some of these were not sensitive to the effects of hypoxia, they were unable to provide insight into the recovery period. Nonetheless, there appears to be persistent cognitive impairments for some standardised cognitive tasks (or domains) and more complex simulated flight performance tasks.

The most informative study designs used repeated measures of simple tasks that have a short temporal resolution, such as reaction speed and attention (refer to Table 2). Initially, Phillips et al. (2009) reported choice reaction time was slower 10 min into recovery, with some participants exhibiting a slower reaction time compared to hypoxia [21]. The same researchers later demonstrated simple and choice reaction speed were slower after 1 and 2 hours into recovery, and normalised at 24 hours [20]; however, there were no measures between 2 and 24 hours, with full recovery likely occurring earlier. More recently, Blacker & McHail (2021) reported vigilant attention (i.e. mean reaction time using a 10 min psychom*otor vigilance task) was reduced 20 min into recovery and normalised after 1 hour, with no differences between breathing 21% or 100% oxygen during recovery [5]. This was similar to an earlier study that reported total response time for simple and choice reaction time tasks normalising by 60 min [17]. These observations suggest that performance of both simple reaction time and more demanding vigilance tasks can be impaired in the hours following a hypoxic exposure.

Performance of complex tasks were less informative as these are more difficult to measure. Since they typically take longer to assess, repeated measures are also difficult to ascertain. Similarly, studies reporting cognitive function for a single timepoint or range of time proximal to the cessation of hypoxia did not provide much insight into cognitive recovery profiles. Nevertheless, auditory serial addition task performance (a measure of attention and working memory) normalised at 90 sec [19], and task time and errors for serial number reading using the King-Devick test normalised at 5 min [25], suggesting rapid recovery. There may also be differences in sensory processing following hypoxia. For example, auditory monitoring tasks may recover at a slower rate compared to visual monitoring, memory and mathematical processing [13]. Future studies should aim to corroborate these findings and discern if there is a difference in post-hypoxia recovery profiles between simple and complex cognitive tasks, and how these are influenced by the hypoxic dose and recovery procedures.

Flight performance tasks are more reflective of the real-world implications posed by post-hypoxia cognitive impairments. Robinson et al. (2018) assessed simulated flight performance in non-pilots concurrently with a time-estimation task during successive hypoxic exposures, but only comparisons between different recovery modalities were possible. Following hypoxic exposure (i.e. normobaric 6.5% oxygen for 5 min), there were no differences in flight and time-estimation task performance between 21% and 13.1% oxygen breathing after 35 min [22]. However, flight performance errors and time-estimation task lapses during recovery with 13.1% oxygen were higher when preceded by hypoxia compared with normoxia, suggesting there was an additive effect from the prior hypoxic exposure [22]. Further, Varis et al. (2019 & 2022) assessed simulated flight performance during a return-to-base landing following an inflight hypoxic emergency on trained Hawk fighter pilots in two studies and reported impairments persisted for 10 min following exposure to 6% oxygen, alongside impaired situational awareness and adverse subjective feelings such as light headedness, visual impairments and dizziness [6, 28]. Some participants also reported incidents when driving home following testing [6].

Recovery of physiological and neurophysiological status

Hypoxia elicits an integrated physiological response that stems primarily from peripheral chemoreceptors sensing hypoxaemia [42]. This chemoreflex is initiated from receptors predominantly located in the carotid body, which increase activity via the carotid sinus nerve that projects to the lower brainstem and nucleus tractus solitarius. This increases ventilation and autonomic sympathetic activity to raise HR and BP, and redistribute blood flow to critical tissues where vasodilation occurs, such as the brain [43]. The subsequent baroreflex initiates a vagal response and there seems to be a baroreflex resetting to shift baroreceptor activity to a higher threshold that allows for sustained and increased sympathetic innervation [36]. In severe hypoxia, compensatory responses are insufficient to maintain brain tissue oxygenation [44], which results in widespread slowing of EEG indices [19]. During normoxic recovery, HR and SpO2 recover within seconds and only marginally faster when breathing 100% oxygen (refer to Table 3). This withdraws the primary chemoreceptor stimulus, yet some physiological perturbations persist.

Autonomic nervous system activity can be inferred using cardiac indices, such as HRV. During hypoxia, HRV indices tend to decline, including SDNN, rMSSD and LF and HF spectral domains (refer to Table 3), indicating greater sympathetic innervation and parasympathetic withdrawal; however, these seem to normalise within 3–20 min [14, 15, 19, 23]. Sex and susceptibility to hypoxia also appear to influence the HRV response. For example, Botek et al. (2015) reported greater vagal withdrawal (i.e. lower HF spectral domain) during hypoxia and at 7 min into recovery in participants exhibiting a lower SpO2 [14]. The same researchers later reported males had a relatively higher sympathetic response to hypoxia exposure compared with females (i.e. higher natural logarithm of SDNN/rMSSD and LF spectral domain), but differences did not persist at 7 min into recovery [15]. Therefore, HRV indices suggest the increase in sympathetic activity during hypoxia normalises minutes into recovery.

Increased sympathetic activity may, in fact, persist for longer than indicated by HRV indices. For example, several included articles also reported increased MSNA for at least 15–20 min into recovery [26, 33, 35, 36, 38]. Although MNSA was not included in this review due to it being a difficult measure to ascertain, it provides a direct measure of sympathetic activity and highlights that increased sympathetic activity during hypoxic recovery may not be fully captured by HRV. The reasons for this are uncertain and despite elevated post-hypoxia MNSA being reduced by periods of hyperoxic breathing to suggest reduced chemoreflex activity [35], the rapid normalisation of SpO2, end-tidal gases and VE indicates chemosensitivity is unlikely underpinning persistent elevated MSNA. Rather, increased MSNA is more likely due to other reasons, such as long-term potentiation of post-ganglionic nerves [35].

The cardiovascular response to hypoxia increases BP, particularly SBP, which appears to normalise within 5–20 min in most, but not all, studies (refer to Table 3). Increased BP initiates the baroreflex to increase vagal activation and during hypoxia there is an attenuated cardiac baroreflex to allow for vagal adaptation. For example, Roche et al. (2002) demonstrated the increased hypoxic sympathetic excitation can be followed by an upregulated parasympathetic drive stemming from overactivity of the baroreflex to cause relative bradycardia during recovery [23]. This reduction in post-hypoxia HR was also shown by Botek et al. (2015 & 2018) [14, 15]. The interaction of arterial carbon dioxide and hypoxia may also alter baroreceptor resetting, with poikilocapnic hypoxia potentially having less influence on baroreceptor resetting compared to isocapnic hypoxia [45]. These effects highlight a complex interplay of chemoreflex and baroreflex regulation during hypoxia and recovery.

Brain tissue oxygenation (e.g. rSO2) is a more informative measure than peripheral indices (e.g. SpO2) of cognitive function [44]. However, studies measuring post-hypoxia rSO2 using near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) report conflicting findings. Phillips et al. (2009 & 2019) demonstrated rSO2 remained reduced at 10 min [21] and 2 hours [20], and normalised by 24 hours into recovery [20]. These perturbations in rSO2 mirrored performance impairments for SRT and CRT tasks [20, 21]. Nonetheless, considering no measures were taken between 2 and 24 hours, rSO2 probably normalised before 24 hours and, as these studies were not adequately controlled, the authors described the findings as preliminary. In comparison, Steinback et al. (2012) and Uchida et al. (2020) reported rSO2 normalised 10–16 min into post-hypoxia recovery [27, 39]. These differences are difficult to explain but are likely due to methodological differences in hypoxic and recovery interventions, and fNIRS measurement techniques, which may not necessarily accurately reflect brain tissue oxygenation [46]. Further, measures of frontal/prefrontal cortex oxygenation represents a tissue average and may not be sensitive to regional differences that can occur during hypoxia.

The ventilatory increase during hypoxia peaks after 5 min then declines over 20–30 min to a steady-state above pre-hypoxic levels [26]. This hyperventilation can cause hypocapnia if carbon dioxide is not administered within the breathing gas (i.e. poikilocapnic hypoxia), which may be an important physiological determinant in studies measuring flight performance during hypoxic recovery [28]. Ventilation typically normalised within 15 min following cessation of hypoxia (refer to Table 4) and, in some studies, this included an initial transient undershoot to below pre-hypoxic levels after returning to normoxia [40] and hyperoxia (compared to hyperoxic baseline) [16]. The ventilatory response to repeated hypoxic exposures may also be attenuated [31], which suggests central chemosensitivity is reduced, which could increase susceptibility to hypoxia by exacerbating hypoxaemia.

EEG measures provide insight into brain signalling activity. Malle et al. (2016) measured EEG waveforms whilst performing a demanding cognitive task (i.e. PASAT) and showed an increase in SEF95 during hypoxia (i.e. the frequency below which 95% of total EEG power was contained), suggesting an increase in fast-wave activity [19]. However, recovery with 100% oxygen breathing generated a robust EEG slowing for ~30 sec (i.e. increase in theta activity and decrease in SEF95) [19], suggesting hyperoxia may elicit an initial harmful effect on the brain. Whereas, Blacker & McHail (2021) measured passive elicited event-related potentials that assess auditory processing, and demonstrated a continued decline in mismatch negativity (MMN) amplitude during post-hypoxia recovery, which normalised after 120 min, and a delayed response MMN peak latency, with shorter latencies that normalised after 240 min [5]. Whereas, P3a, a measure of attention, normalised immediately during recovery, and 100% compared with 21% oxygen breathing had no effect on recovery of EEG indices [5]. Therefore, pre-conscious auditory processing may require up to 2 hours to recover following a hypoxic exposure, which could be connected with the persistent impairment in auditory monitoring previously reported [13].

Perspectives and conclusion

Understanding recovery from hypoxia and its practical implications is critical for various populations. Current research suggests there may be lagging effects for the recovery of some cognitive and physiological indices, but temporal profiles are unclear. Generally, most research has focussed on measuring responses to hypoxia rather than during recovery following return to normoxic and/or hyperoxic breathing. The effects of hypoxia have largely been established and although there is a better need to understand how these affect behaviour and decision making within various real-world situations, further research also needs to consider the post hypoxic period. Impaired cognitive functions can compromise performance and safety, and there is emerging evidence to suggest complex real-world skills, such as piloting an aircraft, are impaired during the immediate minutes following a hypoxic exposure. Thus, if there is the assumption that recovery is rapid due to SpO2 normalising within seconds-to-minutes, then individuals are likely to expose themselves to unnecessary risk during the post-hypoxia period that could result in serious or fatal incidents. This could occur following numerous situations, such as an inflight decompression at high-altitude, failure of oxygen supply systems to provide sufficient oxygen, hypoxia recognition training (i.e. standard military aircrew training), or after hypoxia research studies. However, the duration of potential impairments and the recovery profiles are unclear.

The mediating effects of the hypoxic dose and level of oxygen supplied during recovery is also uncertain. It seems plausible that the longer and more severe a hypoxic exposure, the longer the recovery period. However, whether interactions of FIO2 (and/or barometric pressure) and duration that elicit similar hypoxic doses cause different recovery profiles should also be assessed. For example, studies within this review included prolonged moderate-hypoxia (e.g. ~18,000 ft [5,486 m] equivalent or ~10–11% oxygen) and short severe-hypoxia (e.g. ~25,000 ft [7,620 m] equivalent or ~7–8% oxygen) interventions, but it is unclear which elicits greater cognitive and physiological effects during the post-hypoxic recovery. With poikilocapnic hypoxia, there is also an increased risk of hypocapnia resulting from hyperventilation, which causes cerebral hypoperfusion to exacerbate the effects of hypoxia. Therefore, the mediating effect of adding carbon dioxide to the recovery gas also needs to be examined.

Hyperoxic breathing is currently a focal area of research, particularly in military aviation, and appears to have a beneficial effect on aspects of cognition [47]. Nonetheless, the use of 100% oxygen for recovery from hypoxia did not demonstrate a beneficial effect compared to normoxia. Rather, 100% compared with 21% oxygen breathing during recovery seemed to cause an EEG slowing and impaired cognitive performance during the initial seconds following hypoxia [19], but this was not reported for all studies [5]. This suggests hyperoxic recovery may be harmful, which has previously been eluded to in the military aviation context [4], yet remains standard practice. There may also be a paradoxical effect whereby hyperoxic or nomoxic breathing post-hypoxia elicits a transient worsening of cognitive functions and symptoms, termed the ‘oxygen paradox’ [48]. This is potentially due to a transient vasoconstrictive effect of hyperoxia that reduces cerebral perfusion and a drop in arterial blood pressure [49], thus exacerbating tissue hypoxia. However, to the authors’ knowledge, there is no published literature demonstrating the effects of hyperoxia compared with normoxia on post-hypoxia cerebral perfusion and tissue oxygenation. In fact, there is scant research investigating the oxygen paradox, including cognitive, physiological and perceptive responses.

Whether hypoxia is induced by a reduction in barometric pressure or FIO2 is critical to manage the risk of decompression sickness (DCS). Ascending above 18,000 ft (5,485 m), which is approximately half the atmospheric pressure at sea-level, is associated with an increased risk of venous gas emboli and DCS [50]. Although we did not include DCS risk within our review, this is an important consideration in the aviation context and pose a greater risk than hypoxia alone.

In summary, this systematic review suggests there is a need for post-hypoxia restrictions to minimise the risk of potential incidents and accidents due to lagging cognitive and physiological effects. As such, current evidence supports the presence of a ‘hypoxia hangover’ but the severity and duration of impairments are difficult to quantify and likely depend on several factors. Future research should aim to systematically assess a range of cognitive and physiological responses that persist into recovery following a hypoxic exposure, and the influence of different conditions (e.g. hypoxic dose and recovery procedures). This review suggests that recovery of SpO2 and HR may only indicate partial recovery, and normalisation of other physiological indices can require to up to ~2–4 hours to return to levels before the hypoxic exposure. It therefore seems appropriate that safety measures are implemented following acute hypoxic exposures to mitigate the risks imposed by persistent cognitive impairments and physiological perturbations.

Supporting information

S1 Table

Methodological quality assessment summary.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The views expressed in the manuscript are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy or position of the New Zealand Defence Force or Royal New Zealand Air Force.

Abbreviations

BPBlood pressure
CO2Carbon dioxide
DBPDiastolic blood pressure
DCSDecompression sickness
ECGElectrocardiogram
EEGElectroencephalogram
FICO2Fraction of inspired carbon dioxide
HRHeart rate
HRVHeart rate variability
LF/HFLow-frequency/high-frequency
MAPMean arterial pressure
MMNMismatch negativity component
MSNAMuscle sympathetic nerve activity
NASA-TLXNational Aeronautics and Space Administration-Task Load Index Task load index
O2Oxygen
PASATPaced Auditory Serial Addition Task
PETCO2End-tidal partial pressure of carbon dioxide
PETO2End-tidal partial pressure of oxygen
PO2Partial pressure of oxygen
PRISMAPreferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis
PVTPsychom*otor vigilance task
rMSSDRoot mean square of successive differences between normal heartbeats
rSO2Regional cerebral tissue haemoglobin oxygen saturation
SBPSystolic blood pressure
SDNNStandard deviation of NN intervals
SpO2Peripheral blood haemoglobin-oxygen saturation
SYNWINSynthetic work environment
VEMinute ventilation

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

Data Availability

All data can be found within the manuscript and Supporting information.

References

1. McMorris T, Hale BJ, Barwood M, et al. Effect of acute hypoxia on cognition: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev2017; 74: 225–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.019 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

2. Coppel J, Hennis P, Gilbert-Kawai E, et al. The physiological effects of hypobaric hypoxia versus normobaric hypoxia: a systematic review of crossover trials. Extrem Physiol Med2015; 4. doi: 10.1186/s13728-014-0021-6 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

3. Bailey DM. Oxygen, evolution and redox signalling in the human brain; quantum in the quotidian. Journal of Physiology2019; 597: 15–28. doi: 10.1113/JP276814 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Shaw DM, Cabre G, Gant N. Hypoxic Hypoxia and Brain Function in Military Aviation: Basic Physiology and Applied Perspectives. Front Physiol2021; 12. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.665821 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Blacker KJ, McHail DG. Time course of recovery from acute hypoxia exposure as measured by vigilance and event-related potentials. Physiol Behav2021; 239. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113508 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Varis N, Parkkola KI, Leino TK. Hypoxia hangover and flight performance after normobaric hypoxia exposure in a Hawk simulator. Aerosp Med Hum Perform2019; 90: 720–724. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.5289.2019 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

7. Funke ME, Phillips JB, Seech TR. Differing Oxygen Concentrations and the Effect on Post-Hypoxia Recovery. Naval Medical Research Unit (Dayton), Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, United States.

8. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ2021; 372: 1–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Piet Van Rosendal S, Osborne MA, Fassett RG, et al. Guidelines for Glycerol Use in Hyperhydration and Rehydration Associated with Exercise. Sports Medicine2010; 40: 113–139. doi: 10.2165/11530760-000000000-00000 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

10. Maher CG, Moseley AM, Sherrington C, et al. A Description of the Trials, Reviews, and Practice Guidelines Indexed in the PEDro Database. Physical Therapy; 88. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20080002 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Jadad AR, Andrew Moore R, Carroll D, et al. Assessing the Quality of Reports of Randomized Clinical Trials: Is Blinding Necessary?Control Clin Trials1996; 17: 1–12. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(95)00134-4 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

12. Verhagen AP, De Vet HCW, De Bie RA, et al. The Delphi list: A criteria list for quality assessment of randomized clinical trials for conducting systematic reviews developed by Delphi consensus. J Clin Epidemiol1998; 51: 1235–1241. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(98)00131-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

13. Beer JMA, Shender BS, Chauvin D, et al. Cognitive deterioration in moderate and severe hypobaric hypoxia conditions. Aerosp Med Hum Perform2017; 88: 617–626. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4709.2017 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

14. Botek M, Krejčí J, de Smet S, et al. Heart rate variability and arterial oxygen saturation response during extreme normobaric hypoxia. Auton Neurosci2015; 190: 40–45. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2015.04.001 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Botek M, Krejčí J, McKune A. Sex Differences in Autonomic Cardiac Control and Oxygen Saturation Response to Short-Term Normobaric Hypoxia and Following Recovery: Effect of Aerobic Fitness. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne)2018; 9. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2018.00697 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

16. Dahan A, Berkenbosch A, Degoede J, et al. Influence of hypoxic duration and posthypoxic inspired O2 concentration on short term potentiation of breathing in humans. Journal of Physiology1995; 488: 803–813. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp021012 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Dart T, Gallo M, Beer J, et al. Hyperoxia and hypoxic hypoxia effects on simple and choice reaction times. Aerosp Med Hum Perform2017; 88: 1073–1080. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4696.2017 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

18. Harshman SW, Geier BA, Fan M, et al. The identification of hypoxia biomarkers from exhaled breath under normobaric conditions. J Breath Res2015; 9. doi: 10.1088/1752-7155/9/4/047103 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Malle C, Bourrilhon C, Pierard C, et al. Physiological and Cognitive Effects of Acute Normobaric Hypoxia and Modulations from Oxygen Breathing. Aerosp Med Hum Perform2016; 87: 3–12. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4335.2016 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

20. Phillips JB, Hørning D, Funke ME. Cognitive and perceptual deficits of normobaric hypoxia and the time course to performance recovery. Aerosp Med Hum Perform2015; 86: 357–365. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.3925.2015 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

21. Phillips JB, Simmons RG, Florian JP, et al. Moderate intermittent hypoxia: Effect on two-choice reaction time followed by a significant delay in recovery. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society2009; 53: 1564–1568. [Google Scholar]

22. Robinson FE, Horning D, Phillips JB. Preliminary study of the effects of sequential hypoxic exposures in a simulated flight task. Aerosp Med Hum Perform2018; 89: 1050–1059. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.5052.2018 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

23. Roche F, Reynaud C, Garet M, et al. Cardiac baroreflex control in humans during and immediately after brief exposure to simulated high altitude. Clin Physiol Funct Imaging2002; 22: 301–306. doi: 10.1046/j.1475-097x.2002.00434.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

24. Sausen KP, Bower EA, Stiney ME, et al. A Closed-Loop Reduced Oxygen Breathing Device for Inducing Hypoxia In Humans. Aviat Space Environ Med2003; 74: 1190–1197. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

25. Stepanek J, Cocco D, Pradhan GN, et al. Early detection of hypoxia-induced cognitive impairment using the king-devick test. Aviat Space Environ Med2013; 84: 1017–1022. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. Tamisier R, Anand A, Nieto LM, et al. Arterial pressure and muscle sympathetic nerve activity are increased after two hours of sustained but not cyclic hypoxia in healthy humans. J Appl Physiol2005; 98: 343–349. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00495.2004 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

27. Uchida K, Baker SE, Wiggins CC, et al. A Novel Method to Measure Transient Impairments in Cognitive Function During Acute Bouts of Hypoxia. Aerosp Med Hum Perform2020; 91: 839–844. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.5665.2020 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

28. Varis N, Leinonen A, Parkkola K, et al. Hyperventilation and Hypoxia Hangover During Normobaric Hypoxia Training in Hawk Simulator. Front Physiol2022; 13. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.942249 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

29. Vigo DE, Pérez Lloret S, Videla AJ, et al. Heart Rate Nonlinear Dynamics During Sudden Hypoxia at 8230 m Simulated Altitude. Wilderness Environ Med2010; 21: 4–10. doi: 10.1016/j.wem.2009.12.022 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

30. Janáky M, Grósz A, Tóth E, et al. Hypobaric hypoxia reduces the amplitude of oscillatory potentials in the human ERG. Documenta Ophthalmologica2007; 114: 45–51. doi: 10.1007/s10633-006-9038-5 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

31. Easton PA, Slykerman LJ, Anthonisen NR, et al. Recovery of the ventilatory response to hypoxia in normal adults. J Appl Physiol1988; 64: 521–528. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1988.64.2.521 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

32. Georgopoulos D, Walker S, Anthonisen NR. Effect of sustained hypoxia on ventilatory response to CO2 in normal adults. J Appl Physiol1990; 66: 1071–1078. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1990.68.3.891 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

33. Morgan BJ, Crabtree DC, Skatrud JB, et al. Combined hypoxia and hypercapnia evokes long-lasting sympathetic activation in humans. J Appl Physiol1995; 79: 205–213. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1995.79.1.205 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

34. Najmanová E, Pluháček F, Botek M, et al. Intraocular pressure response to short-term extreme normobaric hypoxia exposure. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne)2019; 10. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2018.00785 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

35. Querido JS, Kennedy PM, Sheel AW. Hyperoxia attenuates muscle sympathetic nerve activity following isocapnic hypoxia in humans. J Appl Physiol2010; 108: 906–912. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01228.2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

36. Querido JS, Wehrwein EA, Hart EC, et al. Baroreflex control of muscle sympathetic nerve activity as a mechanism for persistent sympathoexcitation following acute hypoxia in humans. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol2011; 301: R1779–R1785. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00182.2011 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

37. Stepanek J, Pradhan GN, Cocco D, et al. Acute hypoxic hypoxia and isocapnic hypoxia effects on oculometric features. Aviat Space Environ Med2014; 85: 700–707. doi: 10.3357/asem.3645.2014 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

38. Xie A, Skatrud JB, Puleo DS, et al. Exposure to hypoxia produces long-lasting sympathetic activation in humans. J Appl Physiol2001; 91: 1555–1562. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.91.4.1555 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

39. Steinback CD, Shoemaker JK. Differential regulation of sympathetic burst frequency and amplitude following acute hypoxia in humans. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol2012; 303: R633–R638. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00130.2012 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

40. Bascom DA, Pandit JJ, Clement ID, et al. Effects of different levels of end-tidal PO2 on ventilation during isocapnia in humans. Respir Physiol1992; 88: 299–311. doi: 10.1016/0034-5687(92)90004-g [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

41. Willie CK, Macleod DB, Shaw AD, et al. Regional brain blood flow in man during acute changes in arterial blood gases. Journal of Physiology2012; 590: 3261–3275. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.228551 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

42. López-Barneo J, González-Rodríguez P, Gao L, et al. Oxygen sensing by the carotid body: mechanisms and role in adaptation to hypoxia. Am J Physiol Cell Physiol2016; 310: 629–642. doi: 10.1152/ajpcell.00265.2015 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

43. Ainslie PN, Hoiland RL, Bailey DM. Lessons from the laboratory; integrated regulation of cerebral blood flow during hypoxia. Exp Physiol2016; 101: 1160–1166. doi: 10.1113/EP085671 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

44. Williams TB, Corbett J, McMorris T, et al. Cognitive performance is associated with cerebral oxygenation and peripheral oxygen saturation, but not plasma catecholamines, during graded normobaric hypoxia. Exp Physiol2019; 104: 1384–1397. doi: 10.1113/EP087647 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

45. Cooper VL, Pearson SB, Bowker CM, et al. Interaction of chemoreceptor and baroreceptor reflexes by hypoxia and hypercapnia—A mechanism for promoting hypertension in obstructive sleep apnoea. Journal of Physiology2005; 568: 677–687. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.094151 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

46. Hoiland RL, Griesdale DE, Sekhon MS. Assessing autoregulation using near infrared spectroscopy: more questions than answers. Resuscitation2020; 156: 280–281. doi: 10.1016/j.resuscitation.2020.07.035 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

47. Shaw DM, Bloomfield PM, Gant N. The effect of acute normobaric hyperoxia on cognition: A systematic review, meta-analysis and meta-regression. Physiol Behav2023; 267. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2023.114208 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

48. Latham F.The oxygen paradox experiments on the effects of oxygen in human anoxia. Lancet1951; 257: 77–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

49. G.E. Meadows FJI and JE. A potential explanation of the oxygen paradox. Journal of Physiology 2002; 539P.

50. Webb JT, Pilmanis AA. Fifty years of decompression sickness research at Brooks AFB, TX: 1960–2010. Aviat Space Environ Med2011; 82: A1–A25. doi: 10.3357/asem.2576.2011 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

Recovery from acute hypoxia: A systematic review of cognitive and physiological responses during the ‘hypoxia hangover’ (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Duncan Muller

Last Updated:

Views: 6190

Rating: 4.9 / 5 (79 voted)

Reviews: 86% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Duncan Muller

Birthday: 1997-01-13

Address: Apt. 505 914 Phillip Crossroad, O'Konborough, NV 62411

Phone: +8555305800947

Job: Construction Agent

Hobby: Shopping, Table tennis, Snowboarding, Rafting, Motor sports, Homebrewing, Taxidermy

Introduction: My name is Duncan Muller, I am a enchanting, good, gentle, modern, tasty, nice, elegant person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.